Research summaries
Some research lines I thought were worth providing an overview for:
- Anticipatory automaticity and a reliable spatial attentional bias task
- Attentional carryover: Effects of trial-to-trial history on attentional bias variability
- Freezing as active threat anticipation
- Models of reflectivity and automaticity
Anticipatory automaticity and the predictive Visual Probe Task (cVPT) ↑
The predictive cue-based Visual Probe Task, cVPT or predVPT for short (Gladwin, 2017, but first refined to its current form with separate training phases in Gladwin & Vink, 2018), is a possibly useful variant of the dot-probe task based on predictive cues that provide information about where emotionally salient but task-irrelevant stimuli may appear. Trials on which emotionally salient stimuli (but not probe stimuli) occur are intermixed with trials on which probe stimuli (but not the emotionally salient stimuli) occur. This provides a bias score derived from performance on probe trials that is based on predicted stimulus categories rather than actually-presented exemplars. The bias was termed the anticipatory attentional bias or, more theoretically neutrally, predictive cue-based attentional bias.The two trial types of the task are illustrated below: Picture trials establish (over an explicit training phase, in the current version of the task) the expectation, i.e., of where stimuli from each category may appear; here, blue Os would predict alcohol stimuli, and yellow bars would predict non-alcohol stimuli. Probe trials require a response and provide the behavioural measure of whether attention has been affected by the predictive cues: In the illustration, the probe stimulus appeared at the predicted alcohol location, and would be expected to be relatively fast for riskier drinkers. Note that a non-probe stimulus is presented as well (the VV), since otherwise it seems likely that the basic exogenous attentional process of detecting the appearance of a probe by itself could remove any measurable behavioural effects of salience-driven anticipatory attention.
The use of predictive cues, together with other task features, was expected to reduce undesirable trial-to-trial variation, in particular that caused by which exemplars of stimulus categories happen to have been presented on a particular trial. The cVPT was therefore used to study Attentional Bias Variability (Gladwin & Vink, 2018) involving alcohol stimuli, which was predicted and confirmed to be associated with conflicting automatic associations, measured using dual Single-Target Implicit Association Tests. Further, the anticipatory bias was correlated with risky drinking. The reduction of exemplar-related variation was also thought to potentially improve the reliability of bias scores. Good reliability (around .7 to .8) of the anticipatory bias was indeed found for alcohol (Gladwin, 2019), and individual differences in bias were again associated with risky drinking. A series of follow-up studies (Gladwin, Banic, Figner & Vink, 2020) further explored the nature of the bias and individual differences. One important finding of these studies was the validation of the previously found reliability in terms of predicted stimulus categories rather than cue features, as reliability was very low when the cues were made non-predictive.
Anticipatory spatial attentional bias to threat (Gladwin, Möbius, McLoughlin & Tyndall, 2019) was also found to exist, and this was replicated using an improved procedure (Gladwin, Figner & Vink, 2019) for assessing the reliability of the anticipatory component of the bias that involved reversing the predictive values of the cues. For both studies, this reliability was high when considered in the context of reports of near-zero reliability for traditional tasks and the relatively complex task designs, but nevertheless only modest in psychometric terms; and it was lower, around .4 to .5, than the reliability found for the alcohol-related bias. However, a further study (Gladwin & Vink, 2020) confirmed that when the task and procedure were optimized for reliability, a similar split-half reliability as for the alcohol bias, around .7, could be achieved. Reliability increased to .89 when probe location probability was manipulated to alternate between blocks with a task-induced bias towards versus away from threat (Gladwin, Halls & Vink, 2021). This study also showed that anxiety was associated with stronger avoidance of threat in blocks in which there was a congruent task-induced bias away from threat; that is, it seems that in blocks where there was a task-induced bias towards threat, the influence of individual differences in automatic, emotional avoidance was suppressed. Good reliability of this kind of predictive cues has also been replicated independently, in a three-experiment study that also found associations with anxiety (Basanovic, 2024).
The interpretation of the bias as anticipatory was based on the predicted outcome-based cognitive response selection model (defining "R3-reflectivity", see below) that originally motivated the anticipatory attentional bias work. This was supported in a predictive ABM training study (Gladwin, Möbius & Becker, 2019), in which training towards or away from predicted threat generalized to post-training stimulus-evoked bias; i.e., it's not just about the visual features of the cues becoming salient themselves via conditioning. This approach may also address a potential issue with usual Attention Bias Modification paradigms, namely that even when training attention away from certain stimulus categories, those categories are still task-relevant and therefore being made or kept salient, termed the salience side-effect (Gladwin, 2017).
There are lots of interesting open questions. For instance, the interpretation of effects so far has been relatively equivocal on specific underlying processes, taking an overall "attention as selection for action" perspective. That is: for an effect to be "attentional", it only needs to be caused by the (predicted) spatial positions of salient visual stimuli. For example, it may be that response priming occurs in response to the location of predicted emotional stimuli in a detection task, which could be seen as a problem for interpretation - but such response priming would be caused by biasing action towards certain stimuli and their spatial location, which is subsequently translated into the associated spatial movement; behavioural effects would therefore still be a reflection of attentional bias towards certain stimulus categories as desired. What is indeed unknown is what specific cognitive process most proximally causes reaction time effects, but this is a different, more granular question than whether an attentional bias exists and can be evidenced using a given task design. It may well be that next steps should use psychophysiological measures, like the LRP or lateralized occipital or parietal alpha amplitude, rather than trying to probe component processes via performance and complex task conditions. It might be found that that there's a trade-off between a task with good psychometric properties and a task that attempts to break apart naturally coordinated processes. In other words: Perhaps the starting point for trying to measure attentional bias behaviourally should be a unified "look and move" concept, rather then trying to separate "look" from "move" too soon. (See also below, where using a detection task seems to unveil a very clear trial-to-trial effect.)
The results so far add to the evidence that the well-known psychometric problems with measuring attentional bias using traditional task variants should not lead to premature dismissal of such behavioural measures in general: reliability of the bias can be sufficiently high in principle. There are new tasks out there with various different features (predictive cues being just one example) that promise stronger empirical foundations (Sharpe, Halls & Gladwin, 2022).
Papers on attentional bias and predictive cues:
- Gladwin TE, Halls M, Vink M (2021). Experimental control of conflict in a predictive Visual Probe Task: Highly reliable bias scores related to anxiety. Acta Psychologica, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103357. Link.
- Gladwin TE, Vink M (2020). Spatial anticipatory attentional bias for threat: Reliable individual differences with RT-based online measurement. Consciousness and Cognition, 81, 102930, doi:10.1016/j.concog.2020.102930. Link.
- Gladwin TE, Banic M, Figner B, Vink M (2020). Predictive cues and spatial attentional bias for alcohol: manipulations of cue-outcome mapping. Addictive Behaviors, doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.106247. Link.
- Gladwin TE, Figner B, Vink M (2019). Anticipation-specific reliability and trial-to-trial carryover of anticipatory attentional bias for threat. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, doi:10.1080/20445911.2019.1659801. Link.
- Gladwin TE (2019). Spatial anticipatory attentional bias for alcohol: a preliminary report on reliability and associations with risky drinking. Alcoholism and Drug Addiction, 32(1), 63-70, doi:10.5114/ain.2019.85769. Link.
- Gladwin TE, Möbius M, Becker ES (2019). Predictive Attentional Bias Modification induces stimulus-evoked attentional bias for threat. Europe's Journal of Psychology, 15(3), 479-490, https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v15i3.1633. Link.
- Gladwin TE, Möbius M, McLoughlin S, Tyndall I (2019). Anticipatory versus reactive spatial attentional bias to threat. British Journal of Psychology, doi: 10.1111/bjop.12309. Link.
- Gladwin TE, Vink M (2018). Alcohol-related Attentional Bias Variability and conflicting automatic associations. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 9(2), 1-14, http://dx.doi.org/10.5127/jep.062317. Link.
- Gladwin TE (2017). Attentional bias variability and cued attentional bias for alcohol stimuli. Addiction Research and Theory, 25 (1): 32-38. doi: 10.1080/16066359.2016.1196674. Link.
Attentional carryover: Effects of trial-to-trial history on attentional bias variability ↑
One potential cause of within-subject Attentional Bias Variability - whether this is considered noise or an informative measure in itself - concerns trial-to-trial carryover effects (Gladwin & Figner, 2019). Carryover refers to the dependence of the attentional bias on trial N on the probe location on trial N - 1. This was found to be the case using a so-termed "diagonalised" Visual Probe Task, which was specifically optimized for studying trial-to-trial fluctuations. Effects were found for different colours and for threat-versus-neutral stimuli. Responding to a probe stimulus at the location of a given colour induced an attentional bias towards that colour on the next trial. Carryover for threat versus neutral stimuli was asymmetrical: a bias towards threat versus neutral cue was only found following trials on which the participant responded to a probe at the location of the threat versus neutral cue. This pattern of previous-probe-dependence of the threat-related bias was also found for the anticipatory attentional bias (Gladwin, Figner & Vink, 2019). The effect was subsequently replicated with different facial stimuli and task variants (Gladwin, Jewiss & Vink, 2020). In this study it was also tested whether considering the previous trial's target location could result in more reliable scores even with exemplar-based (rather than predictive cue-based) biases and replicable relationships with various mental health measures, but this was not the case.Some forms of carryover effects may be related to trauma symptoms (Gladwin, 2017), although this study used a traditional dot-probe task that did not show a dependence of the bias on the previous probe location as found using the diagonalized VPT.
Interestingly, work by others also indicates that the standard dot-probe task doesn't capture the effect (Maxwell, Fang & Carlson, 2022). The big difference, I suspect, is that the diagonalized task has strong stimulus-response compatibility - you see the probe at a given location, you respond with a key mapped to that location. In the traditional dot-probe task, in contrast, the response is unrelated to probe location, breaking that link. However, another reason could be that the diagonalized task was designed to remove various kinds of trial-to-trial factors that could interfere with effects, by having disjoint sets of responses and stimulus locations on successive trials.
Papers on carryover:
- Gladwin TE, Jewiss M, Vink M (2020). Attentional bias for negative expressions depends on previous target location: Replicable effect but unreliable measures. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, doi:10.1080/20445911.2020.1805453. Link.
- Gladwin TE, Figner B, Vink M (2019). Anticipation-specific reliability and trial-to-trial carryover of anticipatory attentional bias for threat. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, doi:10.1080/20445911.2019.1659801. Link.
- Gladwin TE, Figner B (2019). Trial-to-trial carryover effects on spatial attentional bias. Acta Psychologica, 196, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.04.006. Link.
- Gladwin TE (2017). Carryover effects in spatial attentional bias tasks and their relationship to subclinical PTSD symptoms. Traumatology, 23(4), 303-308. doi: 10.1037/trm0000121. Link.
Threat anticipation and freezing ↑
Freezing (operationalized as body sway reduction and bradycardia in a threatening context) may be a preparatory, rather than "helpless", state (Gladwin, Hashemi, van Ast & Roelofs, 2016). We used a Virtual Shooting Task to manipulate the ability to prepare defensive responses to threat by making participants either armed or unarmed. Freezing was very strongly related to being armed, and within the armed-condition additionally to the degree of threat. The scientific concept of freezing has to be separated from the idea of "being frozen in fear." The task has been used to study neural effects related to freezing and the freeze-fight transition (e.g., Hashemi et al., 2019). Interactions between threat effects and sleep deprivation were studied using a stop-signal version of the task (van Peer, Gladwin & Nieuwenhuys, 2019). This showed that threat affected impulsivity while sleep deprivation caused a general reduction of accuracy.Freezing effects were further explored behaviourally in a subsequent study on distraction and freeze-terminating stimuli (Gladwin & Vink, 2018). We looked at the effect of only-anticipated versus actual virtual attacks as distractors in an emotional Sternberg task. While a task-irrelevant attack was impending, reaction times on the primary working memory task were slowed; but this appeared to be due to a reversible inhibited state that was released after the attack actually occurred. Note that explanations in terms of distraction would predict that the attack actually occurring should lead to more slowing of responses, opposite to the observed effect.
Threat-induced anticipatory slowing of responses on a threat-unrelated task was confirmed in a subsequent series of studies (Gladwin & Vink, 2020). In these studies visually neutral predictive cues were used as distractors, instead of the previously used face versus no-face cues. This was done to focus on improving the comparability between impending (but not actual) attack versus control (no risk of attack) trials. (Comparisons between anticipated and actual attack could better be made using the task design in the previous study.) These studies further explored temporal dynamics of the response by varying the time between the cue and the probe stimulus, showing that the slowing effect arose after around 600 ms and had decreased by 1200 ms. Note that this contrasts with the threat-induced impulsivity found in, e.g., Gladwin, Möbius & Vink (2019). This minor paradox was interpreted in terms of selective lowering of the response threshold to threat-relevant stimuli.
The figure below provides an example of the reaction time curves from the anticipatory study. Curves are plotted for the conditions Safe, Threat and Attack. The horizontal axis shows the Cue-Stimulus Interval [ms], i.e., the time between the presentation of the cue determining the threat of an attack and, for Safe and Threat trials, the appearance of the probe for the working memory task; on Attack trials, the attack stimulus occured first. The primary interest in this study was in the difference between Safe and Threat trials, when no attack occurred. The figure shows the increase in reaction times due to predicted threat arising around half a second after cue presentation. This followed an initial slowing for both Threat and Safe trials that seems likely to reflect an orienting process.
Papers on freezing:
- Gladwin TE, Vink M (2021). Anticipated attack slows responses in a cued Virtual Attack Emotional Sternberg Task. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 17(1), 31-43, https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.1896. Link.
- Hashemi M, Gladwin TE, Margreet de Valk N, Zhang W, Kaldewaij R, van Ast V, Koch S, Klumpers F, Roelofs K (2019). Neural dynamics of shooting decisions and the switch from freeze to fight. Scientific Reports, 9:4240, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-40917-8. Link.
- Gladwin TE, Vink M (2018). Freeze or forget? Virtual attack effects in an emotional Sternberg task. Europe's Journal of Psychology, 14(2), https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v14i2.1473. Link.
- Gladwin TE, Hashemi MM, van Ast V, Roelofs K (2016). Ready and waiting: Freezing as active action preparation under threat. Neuroscience Letters, 619, 182-188. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2016.03.027. Link.
The R3 "Reflective Cycle" model and computational models of automatic associations ↑
The R3 model is the Reprocessing/Reentrance and Reinforcement model of Reflectivity, or "Reflective Cycle" model. It's an attempt at deconstructing and redefining dual-process models: see section 5 of Gladwin, Figner, Crone & Wiers (2011) and this chapter (Gladwin & Figner, 2014) for an argument why we should talk about emergent states of impulsive versus reflective processing, defined parametrically in terms of response selection search time, rather than processes with particular features or separable systems. The model generated the anticipatory attentional bias line of research and more generally serves to justify but evolve the continued use of traditional dual-process concepts (e.g., in prospective memory; Gladwin, Jewiss, Banic & Pereira, 2020). The "R3" label was used in tribute to the well-known 3D space of real numbers, since it aims to contain theories spanned by a limited set of relevant concepts. The theoretical aim of the model is not to coin new terms or introduce "new" concepts. Rather it simply proposes a theoretical space based on existing concepts grounded in cognitive neuroscience, within which we may be able to better conceptualize what we now model as automatic versus controlled processes. The activation of neural information processing underlying behaviour and cognition is generally time-dependent, and so simply delaying the final selection of a response may change the preferred response and the available information. The principle illustrated by this model is that impulsive versus reflective behavior can be generated by a continuous underlying parameter - how much will you delay? - rather than different types of processes. However, faster processes (e.g., more strongly reinforced associations, or simpler computations) will naturally dominate response selection more after shorter than longer delays. Further, certain situations will naturally teach individuals to respond optimizing speed versus more elaboration - more "emotional" situations will thus tend to be linked to more impulsive processing. This close relationship between neurocognitive processes underlying reflectivity and learning/adaptation to the environment has been argued to play a core role in understanding how a vast array of biological and environmental factors interact to determine the development of self-regulation (Vink et al., 2020).Some of the concepts of the model are illustrated in simulations (Gladwin & Figner, 2022, preprint). The simulations hopefully make some points of the model more concrete: (1) how merely changing a parameter that controls response selection time, in combination with temporal dynamics of response value, can lead to more reflective versus more automatic response selections, and (2) how the learning environment can set this reflectivity parameter via reinforcement, by rewarding or punishing fast responses versus taking the time to reflect. The figure below shows an example of a trial with a typical random walk of activation but a dynamic response threshold. If the decay of the threshold had been slow enough, i.e., "more reflective", the early response (represented by hitting the lower curve) would have been avoided and replaced by the response represented by the upper curve.
While the R3 model aims to help conceptualize the relationship between reflective and automatic processing, existing computational semantic models may be translatable into the "automatic" side of the equation. Neural network language models learn from actual language usage, in large text corpora, and represent meaning in a hidden layer of, e.g., 300 neurons. These representations have been shown to allow vector algebra to be applied to word meaning; in particular, the cosine distance between representations provides a geometric measure of semantic similarity. A possible way to flesh out "automatic associations" between concepts is therefore to consider a stronger association as a closer semantic similarity. Such similarities were found between the presentations of words related to nature and to mental health, in line with a broadly-defined biophilia hypothesis (Gladwin, Markwell & Panno, 2022). Further, in the context of alcohol-valence associations, semantic similarities defined at item level was found to be associated with congruence effects on an Implicit Association Test (IAT) variant (Gladwin, 2022).
(To play around with this kind of associations - work in progress making things more accessible than the original scripts here.)
Theoretically, these relationships suggest that such language models, although coming from a different field, might also serve as models of cognitive associations. Methodologically, the results raise the hypothetical possibility that model-based scores could be used to select stimuli for various aims - stronger similarities for large effect sizes, but weaker ones for reliability.
Papers on theoretical models:
- Gladwin TE (2022). Towards the nature of automatic associations: Item-level computational semantic similarity and IAT-based alcohol-valence associations. Addiction Research and Theory, https://doi.org/10.1080/16066359.2022.2123474. Link.
- Gladwin TE, Figner B (2014). "Hot" cognition and dual systems: Introduction, criticisms, and ways forward. In: Frontiers of Cognitive Psychology Series: Neuroeconomics, Judgment and Decision Making, Eds Wilhelms E, Reyna VF. New York: Psychological Press, 157-180. Link. Extract from author's final draft, online preprint.
- Wiers RW, Gladwin TE (2016). Reflective and impulsive processes in addiction and the role of motivation. Reflective and Impulsive Determinants of Human Behavior, Eds Deutsch R, Gawronski B, Hofmann W. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
- Gladwin TE, Figner BF, Crone EA, Wiers RW (2011). Addiction, adolescence, and the integration of control and motivation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1 (4), 364-376. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2011.06.008. Link.